Memory: how updating what you know about our senses and memory might help strengthen your relationships
Preamble and introduction
Please note: this blog post might be a ‘bit of a ride’. It was born from self-work I’m doing to
adjust how I capture my daily experiences and work with them to build more
reliable memories in the post-craniotomy version of my brain. This work led me to explore related topics
through published journals, in podcasts, and other material I’ve found along
the way. You can read on as I make what
I think are reasonable connections between evolution and natural selection, brain
function and how we experience our surroundings, memory and its fallibility,
and how I believe adjusting my own strategies for learning and remembering as I
live post-craniotomy with brain cancer has left me with some insight that might
help others. If this sounds interesting,
keep reading! I’ll reference sources for
many of the claims within, and you can infer that non-referenced claims imply
that they’re based on my understanding and connection of what I’ve experienced
or learned: please consider where relevant
so you can draw your own conclusions, I’m happy to hear more thoughts in the
comments!
I’ve long been interested in how the brain works and more recently
about how the human brain might have evolved into what it is today. Current discussions about the LONG processes
of natural selection include two common themes amongst others: changes in genes
(mutations) happen quite randomly rather than being a guided event, but are typically
influenced by selective pressures that influence an individual’s ability to
pass that gene on to the next generation, and often involve an underlying benefit
to genes that provide an advantage in ability for the gene’s owner to optimize resource
use and energy expenditure.
Our brain and experience of the world
I thought I had a good understanding of how we experience
the world, which went something like this: our brain works as a complex interconnected
structure of nearly 100 billion neurons (around 80 – 90 billion) that are
grouped into regions that take input from our sensory organs – eyes, ears,
nose, mouth, fingers, feet, skin, and more – to make sense of the external world
then save the memories for later.
We have known for decades that our brain takes time to
process and interpret input. The delay
in our visual processing helps make animated images come to life, where frame
rates faster than about 10 to 12 images per second appear as motion instead of
separate images.
A popular current model shows we experience the world in a more
predictive mode than a reactive one. Instead
of waiting for inputs to be processed moment-by-moment, the brain compares
recent input to years of previous memory and predicts in near-real-time what it
thinks will happen next.
I think I can understand here how this is more energetically
efficient, requiring less effort and energy than a rapid near-real-time processing
of input to react to. It makes sense to
me also that this predictive model would have allowed early humans to avoid danger
(e.g., avoiding an aggressive predator or other physical harm in their environment),
thus helping them survive and raise the next generation. Both the energy efficient model and early survival
benefit link to the evolutionary selection forces I started discussing and help
me to better understand how this modern way of understanding how the brain
works makes intuitive sense. It makes it
easy for me to un-learn what I thought I knew from what I thought and understood
my lived experience had been and make me think more about what this means for the
memories I form in my daily experiences.
How a predictive brain influences memory development
Much of this section is my own speculation as influenced by
what I have read and shared above, what I’ve read about modern understanding of
memory, and the work I’ve been doing to re-learn how to form memories with my
post-surgery brain. I quickly learned
that the way I make new memories has changed and I have been inspired to learn
how to use my new brain. I shared pieces
of how I approach re-learning how to remember in my earlier blog post, My
surgery and recovery period, which for me has been about journaling each
day, curating my google maps timeline to accurately represent where I’ve been
each day, and capturing photos and videos of interesting things to remember each
day. I review these with daily, weekly,
and monthly groupings to learn in the same way as I studied in school, when I thought
repetition was key to building long-lasting memory. I occasionally find memories that just don’t
quite make sense and where I can’t correlate different pieces without finding
conflict, and where I need to re-visit and question the memories I find.
While in school, I learned that repetition can be helpful for
memory, and later learned repetition can intentionally or unintentionally modify,
create, or edit memories to the point that they are no longer accurate: repetition
can behave in unexpected ways.
The amygdala and influencing stress-related hormones such as
β-adrenergic agents, epinephrine, glucocorticoids and more can influence memory
storage
There’s room here to think about how a predictive brain will
encode memories, both episodic memory about events and experiences and semantic
memory about facts and general learned knowledge.
How does this connect to our interpersonal relationships?
I opened by suggesting what I’ve learned might be useful for
others, but you’ve likely noticed now that I’ve rambled on about recent
advances in brain function and memory storage and retrieval understanding from more
of a theory angle without connecting it to daily life and relationships. I hope the connection to our daily lives and
interpersonal relationships isn’t a large jump, and that it’s one that could be
useful as we reflect on moments of conflict or unpleasant interactions.
What I think I’m learning is a clearer model that tells me
our memories are less solid than I thought.
When I think now of times where I’ve disagreed with a friend, or perhaps
had a negative interaction, I’m more able to consider that I’m remembering more
about my brain’s prediction of that interaction than a set of objective facts. My hope is that this current framework and
model of how we experience the world around us and how we then encode and
retrieve the memories can help us revisit those moments with less certainty
about the facts, and to use that to reduce the stress we feel from revisiting
them. We might use it to better
understand disagreements when we think we’re debating facts with a friend, but
where we’re actually comparing two sets of ‘close enough’ predictions instead
of actual events.
I hope also that we can better understand the reality of
loved ones whose outward expressions don’t seem to match what we expect. This might be caused by a mis-match in each
of our brains’ predictions given each of our lived experiences. Many of us will have conversations with people
close to us who experience some form of cognitive decline, and my hope is that
a better understanding of how we build and remember our reality might make it
easier for use to relate better to an unfamiliar or illogical reality.
I’m closing on an optimistic and rather ‘fuzzy’ note and know
that those who know me well won’t be surprised.
In finding ways to help myself with my own situation, it’s driven me to
read more into things I’m excited and interested in, and my hope is that at
least a few readers will be interested in the ideas also.
Thanks for reading along, and please do share your thoughts
in the comments!
Works Cited
1. Positive and Negative Selection on the Human
Genome. Fay, Justin C., Wyckoff, Gerald J. and Wu, Chung-I. 3,
2001, Genetics, Vol. 158, pp. 1227-1234.
2. Selection in
Nature: Experimental Manipulations of Natural Populations'. Reznick,
David N. and Ghalambor, Cameron K. 3, 2005, Integrative and Comparative
Biology, Vol. 45, pp. 456-462.
3. Fabian, Daniel
and Flatt, Thomas. The Evolution of Aging. Nature Publishing Group. [Online]
[Cited: 2 29, 2024.]
http://www.nature.com/scitable/knowledge/library/the-evolution-of-aging-23651151.
4. Module 10:
Neuronal Signalling. Berridge, Michael J. 2012, Biochemical
Journal, Vol. 6.
5. Patch-Seq
Protocol to Analyze the Electrophysiology, Morphology and Transcriptome of
Whole Single Neurons Derived From Human Pluripotent Stem Cells. Hurk,
Mark van den, et al. 2018, Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience, Vol. 11,
p. 261.
6. Construction of
the human forebrain. Jernigan, Terry L. and Stiles, Joan. 2017,
Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, Vol. 8.
7. Tracking How
Memories Form. Wolf, Lauren K. 2015, Chemical & Engineering
News.
8. The remarkable,
yet not extraordinary, human brain as a scaled-up primate brain and its
associated cost. Herculano-Houzel, Suzana. 2012, Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 109, pp.
10661-10668.
9. Group, Gamma,
Read, Paul and Meyer, Mark-Paul. Restoration of motion picture film. s.l. :
Butterworth-Heinemann, 2000. pp. 24–26.
10. Schuler, Romana
Karla. Seeing Motion: A History of Visual Perception in Art and
Science. s.l. : Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG.
11. Glycinergic
neurons in the human retina. Frederick, Jeanne M., Rayborn, Mary E. and
Hollyfield, Joe G. 2, 1984, The Journal of Comparative Neurology, Vol.
227, pp. 159-172.
12. The
organization of the retina and visual system. Schmolesky, M.
13. A Comparison of
Visual and Auditory Motion Processing in Human Cerebral Cortex. Lewis,
James W., Beauchamp, Michael S. and DeYoe, Edgar A. 9, 2000, Cerebral
Cortex, Vol. 10, pp. 873-888.
14. The effect of
non-visual working memory load on top-down modulation of visual processing. Rissman,
Jesse, et al. 7, 2009, Neuropsychologia, Vol. 47, pp. 1637-1646.
15. The human
visual system is optimised for processing the spatial information in natural
visual images. Parraga, CA, Troscianko, Tom and Tolhurst, David J.
1, 2000, Current Biology, Vol. 10, pp. 35-38.
16. Attention and
awareness in stage magic: turning tricks into research. Macknik,
Stephen L., et al. 11, 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 9, pp.
871-879.
17. Top-Down
Predictions in the Cognitive Brain. Kveraga, Kestutis, Ghuman, Avniel
Singh and Bar, Moshe. 2, 2007, Brain and Cognition, Vol. 65, pp. 145-168.
18. Not all
repetition is alike: Different benefits of repetition in amnesia and normal
memory. Verfaellie, Mieke, et al. 3, 2008, Journal of The
International Neuropsychological Society, Vol. 14, pp. 365-372.
19. Repetition
Suppression and Multi-Voxel Pattern Similarity Differentially Track Implicit
and Explicit Visual Memory. Ward, Emily J., Chun, Marvin M. and Kuhl,
Brice A. 37, 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 33, pp. 14749-14757.
20. Repetition and
laterality effects on recognition memory for words and pictures. Juola,
James F. 2, 1973, Memory & Cognition, Vol. 1, pp. 183-192.
21. Involvement of
the amygdala in memory storage: Interaction with other brain systems. McGaugh,
James L., Cahill, Larry and Roozendaal, Benno. 24, 1996, Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Vol. 93, pp.
13508-13514.
22. Effects of
Aging, Message Repetition, and Note-Taking on Memory for Health Information. Morrow,
Daniel G., et al. 6, 1999, Journals of Gerontology Series B-psychological
Sciences and Social Sciences, Vol. 54, p. 369.
23. On the dual
effects of repetition on false recognition. Benjamin, Aaron S. 4,
2001, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol.
27, pp. 941-947.
24. Repetition
effects in associative false recognition: Theme-based criterion shifts are the
exception, not the rule. Starns, Jeffery J., Hicks, Jason L. and Marsh,
Richard L. 6, 2006, Memory, Vol. 14, pp. 742-761.
25. Observer
memories may not be for everyone. Radvansky, Gabriel A. and Svob,
Connie. 5, 2019, Memory, Vol. 27, pp. 647-659.
26. Episodic and
semantic autobiographical memory and everyday memory during late childhood and
early adolescence. Willoughby, Karen A., et al. 2012, Frontiers in
Psychology, Vol. 3, pp. 53-53.
Comments
Post a Comment